Force Science News #272:

Needed: A national campaign to attack dangerous deadly force myths

From the entire Force Science team: We wish all of you a healthy and safe new year. We remain grateful for the honor of working with you and humbled by your continued dedication to the mission of your work. That, in fact, fuels a tireless dedication to ours. Thank you for what you do. Onward to 2015!

 

In this edition:

 

I. Needed: A national campaign to attack dangerous deadly force myths

II. What appeals court says cops should know about prone positioning

III. More on world's first study of body-worn cameras in action

 

I. Needed: A national campaign to attack dangerous deadly force myths

 

FORCE SCIENCE NEWS READERS: What do you think of this reader's call for action? Please send comments to: editor@forcescience.org. We'll publish representative responses.

 

In the last few minutes, today's Twitter includes stories on police killing suspects in Utah at a higher rate than other types of homicides...a father asking why his son wasn't Tasered instead of being shot twice...outrage over a man who had shot at police being shot himself (amazing, as it seems so clear cut!)...and a man being shot in Vancouver (BC) for attacking police with a 2 x 4.

 

Logic and reason are being thrown out the window because rhetoric and emotion have been turned up to a degree I believe is unprecedented in history.

 

I can't help but wonder if law enforcement couldn't put together a web portal and social media campaign that explain for civilians and media the issues that Force Science has studied, to help them understand what we understand.

 

The message has to be simple enough to produce sound bites and easy stories for reporters, and it has to explain things in a way lay people can understand. The public has been conditioned for so long to think police shootings play out like TV shows, and I fear that younger generations are even more misinformed due to their tendency to obtain all of their knowledge via social media and uninformed peers.

 

Regular people need to see for themselves how violence plays out in real life, how fast things happen, how people and police officers can be killed by unarmed attackers, how unreliable eyewitness accounts can be, how video doesn't always tell the whole story, the dangers of edged weapons, how people don't necessarily fall to the ground when shot, and how lower levels of force aren't the right choice for deadly encounters. We can go on and on about myths.

 

The lack of public knowledge is alarming. I can see laws being passed in the current climate that will make our jobs more dangerous. It is not inconceivable that a jurisdiction could mandate officers to exhaust all less-lethal means before using deadly force. This would be outrageously stupid and unrealistic but that has never stopped a legislative body.

 

The only way I see to stop this is to have a nationally coordinated response to better educate our politicians, reporters, and the public. Thinking like a PR person would not be a bad thing right now. How great would it be to see stories in newspapers everywhere and story lines in the myriad of CSI-type shows that address these issues. There should be so much information going out in a loud and clear fashion that everyone knows these issues like they know the Miranda warnings.

 

If there is any way to coordinate such a response I would be happy to help out in any way possible.

 

Lt. Glen Mills

Pres., Massachusetts Assn. of Crime Analysts

Burlington (MA) PD

 

Thoughts? Please send comments to: editor@forcescience.org. We'll publish representative responses.

 

II. What appeals court says cops should know about prone positioning

 

The issue of prone positioning of resistant suspects, which we reported on in FSN #270, is back in the news--this time with a US appellate court weighing in with comments on what officers are expected to know about the subject.

 

The case, Bolick v. City of East Grand Rapids (MI), is a civil rights action against police, alleging the use of excessive force in the excited delirium death of a mentally ill and highly combative 30-year-old who had been "freaking out" and threatening to kill his father.

 

When a sergeant and an officer arrived at the scene in response to the father's 911 call for help, the "out of control" son quickly became a veritable poster child for resistance.

 

He punched the officer in the face, ran off, "sucker punched" him again when caught, promptly stood up unfazed after being Tasered, refused to follow directives, broke off the Taser wires after being zapped again, ran off again, withstood more Taser deployments by both the sergeant and the officer, ended up in the kitchen of his home where he "crouched into a fighting position" within easy access to a set of knives, charged at the officer and punched him in the chest, was drive-stunned, and finally, despite "significant resistance," was wrestled to the floor and handcuffed.

 

The suspect, incidentally, was 5'4" and weighed 128 lbs.

 

Despite police statements to the contrary, family members who witnessed the extended confrontation and the suspect's on-scene death soon after EMS arrived claimed that once he was handcuffed in the prone position he no longer offered active resistance. Yet, they insisted, the sergeant continued to apply "pressure to [his] back with both knees and the full weight of his body" and the patrol officer drive-stunned him again in his back.

 

In the civil suit brought by the suspect's father, the district court ruled against summary judgment for the officers and stated that "genuine issues of material fact" regarding the back pressure and the final Tasering require a trial.

 

The federal appellate court for the 6th circuit, considering the defendants' appeal, agreed. In a written decision last September, Judge Edmund Sargus Jr. noted that a jury "could find that the officers' conduct was unreasonable" under the circumstances.

 

He wrote that a reasonable officer would know that it was well-established at the time of the occurrence that "it was excessive...to apply the weight of his body to the back of a handcuffed suspect who did not resist, all while the suspect lay on his stomach with another officer controlling his legs.... [P]utting substantial or significant pressure on a suspect's back while that suspect is in a face-down prone position after being subdued and/or incapacitated constitutes excessive force...."

 

Likewise, the Tasering "under substantially similar conditions was also excessive."

 

The court ordered the case returned to the district level for further proceedings.

 

Sargus mentioned in a footnote that video of the incident might have clarified the dispute "as to whether and what extent" the suspect continued to struggle after being handcuffed. The camera on the officer's Taser was recording but "much of the video came from the vantage point of the Taser placed on the floor next to [the suspect's] body [and] does not resolve" the issue, he wrote.

 

The appellate decision can be accessed in full, free of charge, by clicking here.

 

Our thanks to Atty. Michael Brave, member/manager of LAAW International LLC, for keeping us updated on judicial developments law enforcement and use of force.

 

III. More on world's first study of body-worn cameras in action

 

A groundbreaking study of body-worn cameras first reported by Force Science News 18 months ago has now been published in a professional journal, with additional commentary.

 

An abstract of the research can be accessed free of charge, with an option to buy the full study, by clicking here, which will take you to the website for the Journal of Quantitative Criminology. The essence of the report is also available free by clicking here.

 

The research comprised a yearlong experiment conducted at the Rialto (CA) PD, a mid-size agency that serves a population of 100,000 residents, more than half of them minorities. The city, home to a number of large-scale hard goods warehousers and shippers, experiences an above-average crime rate, with a homicide rate that is nearly 50% higher than the US average. More than 20% of the population lives below the federal poverty line.

 

For 12 months, Rialto's 54 frontline officers all were assigned randomly to wear or not wear Taser HD Axon Flex video/audio cameras attached to their clothing during each of their 12-hr. shifts. On shifts when they wore cameras, "the officers were instructed to have them on during every encounter with members of the public, with the exception of incidents involving sexual assaults of minors and dealing with police informants," the study team explains.

 

Nearly 1,000 shifts around the clock were monitored in all, and all participating officers experienced both camera and non-camera working conditions.

 

Recorded video was automatically uploaded at the end of each tour, and the research team had full access to this "rich" database in what they claim was the world's first test of the effect of body cams on police-subject interactions.

 

The results were dramatic.

 

For three years prior to the experiment, the PD posted roughly 65 use-of-force incidents per year. (UOF was considered "physical force that is greater than basic control or 'compliance holds' " and included OC spray, baton strikes, TASER deployment, K9 bites, or firearms.) In the year before the experiment, 24 citizens lodged grievances against officers.

 

During the experimental period, the UOF rate dropped significantly, to 25 incidents total, a reduction of 58% to 64% compared to previous years. Only eight of the incidents occurred when officers were wearing body cams. In other words, during the test period the likelihood of force being used was roughly doubled when cameras were not deployed.

 

Citizen complaints plunged to a total of 3, a precipitous drop of 88%.

 

The research team's commentary on these findings includes these observations:

 

• Extensive research shows that people tend to "adhere to social norms and change their conduct" once they're aware that their behavior is being observed. Under camera scrutiny, they "become more conscious that unacceptable behaviors will be captured on film," with detection "perceived as certain." Body-worn cameras (BWCs) convey a "straight-forward, pragmatic message: 'You are being watched, videotaped, and expected to follow the rules.' "

 

• This "self-awareness effect" caused by the camera's "neutral third eye" affects the psyches of officers and suspects alike, prompting suspects to "cool down" aggressive actions and deterring officers "from reacting with excessive or unnecessary force." Neither party wants to "get caught engaging in socially undesirable behavior that may have costly consequences."

 

• "[E]ven police subcultures of acceptable but illegitimate force responses are likely to be affected by the cameras," the researchers write, "because misconduct cannot go undetected.... Police-public encounters become more transparent and the curtain of silence that protects misconduct can more easily be unveiled...." On the other hand, cameras may "force" officers "to endure stressful situations and arguably accept some forms of disrespect that without the cameras [they] would normally not."

 

• The study revealed an interesting "spillover effect." Overall, the use of force and citizen complaints declined both when cameras were in use and when they weren't, the researchers point out. They speculate that this may reflect a conscious effort by officers without cameras during a given shift to competitively improve their behavior to favorably match that of fellow officers who had the "advantage" of wearing a body cam.

 

• There's a "training potential" in body-worn videos. The footage "can be used to 'coach' officers about how they conduct themselves," the researchers write. "We envision future police training to incorporate one-on-one sessions in which junior officers train with their own footage...and potentially improve their demeanor when dealing with suspects, victims, and witnesses."

 

• While the researchers consider a cost/benefit analysis to favor BWCs, they acknowledge that one "price" is presently unclear: "What are the direct and indirect costs of storing, sharing, and managing digital evidence?" As the "velocity and volume of data" grow exponentially over time, "user licenses, storage space, security costs, maintenance, and system upgrades can potentially translate into billions of dollars worldwide."

 

• However, "the cost of not having video footage may have direct implications on decisions to prosecute or on criminal proceedings more generally." Historically, testimony by officers in court against defendants has "carried tremendous weight.... Yet it is very likely that defense attorneys, judges, the jury, and the public as a whole would steadfastly assign more weight to digital evidence.... [W]ould district attorneys...be reluctant to prosecute when there is no evidence from body-worn devices to corroborate the testimony of the officer... Would cases be dismissed if arrests or stop-and-frisk were conducted without a body-worn video, given the possible violation of human rights? Will officers' credibility in court be assumed to be violated when police-public encounters are not recorded?"

 

Time will tell whether such potential consequences will prove to offset the benefits of BWCs, the researchers say.

 

Meantime, "mistrust and a lack of confidence...already characterize some communities' perception of their local police," they note. "It may take just one or a handful of cases" of unnecessary or excessive force to further "damage the legitimacy of the police" and inflict "tremendous costs" on agencies already struggling financially "in an era of austerity....

 

"Completely eradicating illegitimate use-of-force is unlikely, as some force will always be required against some offenders in some circumstances. Any approach should still allow for legitimate use-of-force...but for all other circumstances, a reductionist approach should aim to dramatically 'cool down' encounters."

 

Based on the findings of this study, which the research team urges other departments to replicate in their own jurisdictions, BWCs appear to be a potentially valuable tool in achieving that goal, while "enhancing police legitimacy and transparency, increasing prosecution rates, and improving evidence capture...."

 

The researchers authoring the study are William Farrar, chief of Rialto PD, and Drs. Barak Ariel and Alex Sutherland of the Institute of Criminology at the University of Cambridge in England.

 

Our original report on this research, based in large part on an interview with Chief Farrar, who directly managed the experiment, can be accessed by clicking here or by typing the following address for the Force Science News Archives in to your browser: www.forcescience.org/fsnews/230.html

© 2017 Force Science Institute Ltd.