fbpx

Is Prone Positioning Really Riskier For Suspects?

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

An exaggeration of the sudden in-custody death problem is generating “persecution and prosecution” of LEOs and their agencies and is resulting in “reactionary changes in policy and procedure that may well be based in conjecture rather than fact,” according to new findings by a Canadian research team.

In particular the study group challenges the widely held belief that simply placing a subject face-down in a prone position after a use of force creates a substantial threat to life. In fact, the only subject who died in more than 1,200 consecutive force encounters that the team meticulously analyzed was lying on his side, a position commonly advocated for its presumed safety.

“[S]udden in-custody death,” the study concludes, “has more to do with the features of the individual” than with his or her positioning. Even among suspects supposed to be at high risk—the drug and alcohol intoxicated and those with mental illness—there appears to be no scientific basis for believing that prone positioning is as dangerous as its reputation suggests, the researchers report.

“The true understanding of use-of-force events will come from comprehensive, scientific protocols such as this one, which was carried out under the rigorous conditions of a scientific medical study and supervised by the Institutional Review Boards of the universities involved,” Hall told Force Science News.

“Rather than focusing solely on 1 type of force or another, the goal of the study is to evaluate all use-of-force events to better understand which subjects and situations may represent particular risk.”

The investigative team was headed by Dr. Christine Hall, an emergency room physician and a medical faculty member at the University of British Columbia and the University of Calgary. An internationally recognized expert on excited delirium syndrome, Hall is also on the faculty for the certification course in Force Science Analysis conducted by the Force Science Institute.

ORIGIN OF CONTROVERSY

Concern about the relationship between suspect positioning and death first arose in the early 1980s after a medical examiner in Washington State analyzed 3 cases in which prisoners were transported while hogtied and concluded that such positioning was associated with sudden in-custody death because of the suffocating effect on breathing and the inability of the subjects to shift to a different position.

More articles warning about “positional asphyxia” and “restraint asphyxia” followed in medical journals and law enforcement publications and positioning became a widespread training and policy issue.

Hall points out that the medical examiner’s original work on the effects of positioning was shown to be “fraught with methodological errors,” and that other, more sophisticated studies disputed alleged adverse findings. Nonetheless, the debate about suspect positioning “gradually translated into the unsupported idea that any and all prone positioning for any length of time is immediately dangerous.”

Moreover, prone positioning came to be regarded “without scientific evidence” as “particularly dangerous” to restrained suspects who are “under the influence of intoxicants such as alcohol and/or stimulants, experiencing mental distress, or in the agitated, incoherent state known as excited delirium,” as well as those who have been Tasered.

“When a cause of death cannot be found otherwise, positional asphyxia is often suggested to have [played] a role…,” the researchers note.

As the bias against any form of prone positioning intensified, Hall perceived that what was lacking in the debate was a broad-based, detailed examination of real-life incidents to determine exactly what variety of physical positions restrained suspects end up in after force encounters with police and what association there might be between each of these positions and ultimate unfavorable outcomes.

FORCE POOL

With the cooperation of police executives in an unnamed Canadian city with more than 1,100,000 population and nearly 2,000 street cops, Hall and her team collected a vast statistical database across a recent 3-year period. (The city is anonymous in their report to protect privacy, an ethical consideration required by the universities involved.)

Without officers being aware of the researchers’ interests, certain study-related entries were embedded in the department’s standard electronic form that must be filled out after any use of force above soft-hand control. Among other things, officers had to specify the apparent cognitive condition of the subjects they encountered and their “final resting position…once physical control had been achieved…and while awaiting further disposition”: i.e., either prone (face-down) or not prone (face-up, side-lying, sitting, kneeling, or standing). The department involved does not specify a favored position in policy, so there was no motive for officers to shade their responses, Hall points out.

Forms involving suspects at least 18 years old and all “general police duty” uses of force beyond merely “a bent wrist or straightened elbow to gain compliance” were then parsed for particulars. “We did not pick and choose between subjects, events, or outcomes,” Hall says, “but included all uses of force over 3 consecutive years.”

The pool the researchers analyzed ended up being 88% males. Some 40% appeared drunk at the time of police contact; 10% were judged to be drug-intoxicated; 9% seemed mentally distressed; and 28% demonstrated some combinations of those states.

In all, out of 1,566,908 interactions between police and subjects across the study’s 3 years, force was used in only 1,269 contacts.”[C]ontrary to current suggestion that police use of force is rampant, 99.92% of all police-public interactions [recorded by the agency studied] did not involve police use of force,” the researchers report. “This finding did not vary across 3 years of study.”

RISK OUTCOMES

In terms of positioning, nearly 43% of subjects ended up prone in their final resting pose after their force encounter. About 57% were not prone. All were handcuffed, but none was leg- or ankle-restrained or hogtied. “[E]ven though prone positioning was very common,” the researchers noted, none of the “hundreds of subjects in the prone position” died.

Indeed, with only 1 death [a not-prone subject] discovered in the entire study, statistically “there was no [significant] difference in the death rate between prone and not-prone positions.”

Suspects with drug/alcohol intoxication, mental illness, or some combination thereof were essentially evenly distributed between prone and not-prone positioning. Despite the prevalent suspicion that such individuals are at greater risk, “no subject died in the prone position even with a large number of abnormally behaving individuals” in the force pool.

Of the large number of subjects studied, about 29 percent of those who ended up prone had been Tasered, and of those who ended up not prone 25 percent had been Tasered. No deaths were recorded in the prone group.”
The 1 subject who died during the study “was an individual who was assessed by officers on the scene as having both drug intoxication and mental distress, had undergone a single contact stun exposure with a CEW, and had many abnormal characteristics before and after police involvement at the scene,” the study report says. “This subject was placed in the side-lying position at the conclusion of the use-of-force event, prior to cardiopulmonary collapse.

“[He] displayed multiple features of excited delirium. The details…are strikingly similar to other in-custody death occurrences both in and out of the prone position and…with and without CEW application.”

BOTTOM LINE

Hall points out that this is believed to be the “only study to document the incidence of sudden in-custody death across all use-of-force modalities” in a major urban police agency.

Besides dispelling some persistent myths about the risk of prone positioning, she feels the findings are important for confirming the overall rarity of sudden in-custody death. It’s “profoundly low in the real-world environment of police use of force which includes many abnormally behaving individuals in varied circumstances.”

Media publicity of in-custody deaths without an appreciation of their context “results in the presumption of a falsely elevated prevalence” of these occurrences, she writes in the study report. “This, in turn, generates persecution and prosecution of individual police officers and police agencies at great personal and societal cost, fear and mistrust for the police in the public eye, and results in reactionary changes in policy and procedure that may well be based in conjecture rather than fact.”

RECOMMENDATIONS

Hall acknowledges that all research studies have some limitations. Because hogtying was not used in any of the encounters her team studied, the researchers could not assess its specific danger to prone subjects, if any, for example. Also, while they were aware that “weight force” was very likely applied to the back of some of the proned subjects, they were not able to determine how often that occurred. That variable will be evaluated in the future.

“[R]egardless of the low incidence of sudden in-custody death, further study is required,” the team reports. Hall’s objective is to “determine what, if anything, can predict sudden in-custody death in order to improve the outcomes for people undergoing police use of force.”

Meanwhile, the researchers caution that suspects restrained in a prone position—any position, actually—should not be abandoned “for protracted lengths of time.” Suspects always bear close observation.

“[T]he best way for non-medically trained pre-hospital personnel to monitor the [well-being] status of any human being is through observation of the face…,” the researchers suggest. Therefore it is “advisable to assign 1 officer to monitor the restrained subject’s face for signs of distress/difficulty.”

This study was funded by the Canadian Police Research Center. The results appear in the latest issue the Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine, under the title “Incidence and outcome of prone positioning following police use of force in a prospective, consecutive cohort of subjects.” Click here for a brief abstract that’s is available free of charge. The full 7-page study paper can be ordered for a fee at that site.

[Our thanks to Chris Lawrence of the Ontario Police College and a member of the Force Science certification faculty for alerting us to the publication of this study.]

GDPR

  • Privacy Policy

Privacy Policy

Effective date: January 06, 2019

Force Science Institute, Ltd. (“us”, “we”, or “our”) operates the https://www.forcescience.org/ website (hereinafter referred to as the “Service”).

This page informs you of our policies regarding the collection, use, and disclosure of personal data when you use our Service and the choices you have associated with that data. Our Privacy Policy for Force Science Institute, Ltd. is based on the Privacy Policy Template from Privacy Policies.

We use your data to provide and improve the Service. By using the Service, you agree to the collection and use of information in accordance with this policy. Unless otherwise defined in this Privacy Policy, the terms used in this Privacy Policy have the same meanings as in our Terms and Conditions, accessible from https://www.forcescience.org/

Information Collection And Use

We collect several different types of information for various purposes to provide and improve our Service to you.

Types of Data Collected

Personal Data

While using our Service, we may ask you to provide us with certain personally identifiable information that can be used to contact or identify you (“Personal Data”). Personally identifiable information may include, but is not limited to:

  • Email address
  • First name and last name
  • Phone number
  • Address, State, Province, ZIP/Postal code, City
  • Cookies and Usage Data

Usage Data

We may also collect information on how the Service is accessed and used (“Usage Data”). This Usage Data may include information such as your computer’s Internet Protocol address (e.g. IP address), browser type, browser version, the pages of our Service that you visit, the time and date of your visit, the time spent on those pages, unique device identifiers and other diagnostic data.

Tracking & Cookies Data

We use cookies and similar tracking technologies to track the activity on our Service and hold certain information.

Cookies are files with small amount of data which may include an anonymous unique identifier. Cookies are sent to your browser from a website and stored on your device. Tracking technologies also used are beacons, tags, and scripts to collect and track information and to improve and analyze our Service.

You can instruct your browser to refuse all cookies or to indicate when a cookie is being sent. However, if you do not accept cookies, you may not be able to use some portions of our Service. You can learn more how to manage cookies in the Browser Cookies Guide.

Examples of Cookies we use:

  • Session Cookies. We use Session Cookies to operate our Service.
  • Preference Cookies. We use Preference Cookies to remember your preferences and various settings.
  • Security Cookies. We use Security Cookies for security purposes.

Use of Data

Force Science Institute, Ltd. uses the collected data for various purposes:

  • To provide and maintain the Service
  • To notify you about changes to our Service
  • To allow you to participate in interactive features of our Service when you choose to do so
  • To provide customer care and support
  • To provide analysis or valuable information so that we can improve the Service
  • To monitor the usage of the Service
  • To detect, prevent and address technical issues

Transfer Of Data

Your information, including Personal Data, may be transferred to — and maintained on — computers located outside of your state, province, country or other governmental jurisdiction where the data protection laws may differ than those from your jurisdiction.

If you are located outside United States and choose to provide information to us, please note that we transfer the data, including Personal Data, to United States and process it there.

Your consent to this Privacy Policy followed by your submission of such information represents your agreement to that transfer.

Force Science Institute, Ltd. will take all steps reasonably necessary to ensure that your data is treated securely and in accordance with this Privacy Policy and no transfer of your Personal Data will take place to an organization or a country unless there are adequate controls in place including the security of your data and other personal information.

Disclosure Of Data

Legal Requirements

Force Science Institute, Ltd. may disclose your Personal Data in the good faith belief that such action is necessary to:

  • To comply with a legal obligation
  • To protect and defend the rights or property of Force Science Institute, Ltd.
  • To prevent or investigate possible wrongdoing in connection with the Service
  • To protect the personal safety of users of the Service or the public
  • To protect against legal liability

Security Of Data

The security of your data is important to us, but remember that no method of transmission over the Internet, or method of electronic storage is 100% secure. While we strive to use commercially acceptable means to protect your Personal Data, we cannot guarantee its absolute security.

Service Providers

We may employ third party companies and individuals to facilitate our Service (“Service Providers”), to provide the Service on our behalf, to perform Service-related services or to assist us in analyzing how our Service is used.

These third parties have access to your Personal Data only to perform these tasks on our behalf and are obligated not to disclose or use it for any other purpose.

Analytics

We may use third-party Service Providers to monitor and analyze the use of our Service.

  • Google AnalyticsGoogle Analytics is a web analytics service offered by Google that tracks and reports website traffic. Google uses the data collected to track and monitor the use of our Service. This data is shared with other Google services. Google may use the collected data to contextualize and personalize the ads of its own advertising network.You can opt-out of having made your activity on the Service available to Google Analytics by installing the Google Analytics opt-out browser add-on. The add-on prevents the Google Analytics JavaScript (ga.js, analytics.js, and dc.js) from sharing information with Google Analytics about visits activity.For more information on the privacy practices of Google, please visit the Google Privacy & Terms web page: https://policies.google.com/privacy?hl=en

Links To Other Sites

Our Service may contain links to other sites that are not operated by us. If you click on a third party link, you will be directed to that third party’s site. We strongly advise you to review the Privacy Policy of every site you visit.

We have no control over and assume no responsibility for the content, privacy policies or practices of any third party sites or services.

Children’s Privacy

Our Service does not address anyone under the age of 18 (“Children”).

We do not knowingly collect personally identifiable information from anyone under the age of 18. If you are a parent or guardian and you are aware that your Children has provided us with Personal Data, please contact us. If we become aware that we have collected Personal Data from children without verification of parental consent, we take steps to remove that information from our servers.

Changes To This Privacy Policy

We may update our Privacy Policy from time to time. We will notify you of any changes by posting the new Privacy Policy on this page.

We will let you know via email and/or a prominent notice on our Service, prior to the change becoming effective and update the “effective date” at the top of this Privacy Policy.

You are advised to review this Privacy Policy periodically for any changes. Changes to this Privacy Policy are effective when they are posted on this page.

Contact Us

If you have any questions about this Privacy Policy, please contact us:

  • By email: support@forcescience.org
  • By visiting this page on our website: https://www.forcescience.org/contact
  • By phone number: 866-683-1944
  • By mail: Force Science Institute, Ltd.